The Milgram experiment 1963 stands as one of the most influential and controversial psychological studies ever conducted, fundamentally challenging our understanding of human obedience to authority. Stanley Milgram’s groundbreaking research at Yale University revealed shocking truths about ordinary people’s willingness to inflict harm when instructed by authority figures, results that continue to resonate in psychological research and ethical debates today.

Table of Contents
The Historical Context Behind the Milgram Experiment 1963
Stanley Milgram, a Yale University psychologist, designed the Milgram experiment 1963 in the aftermath of World War II and the Holocaust. Originally intending to compare obedience levels between Americans and Germans, Milgram suspected that unique cultural factors in Germany might explain higher submission to authority. However, when he conducted baseline testing with American participants, the results were so unexpectedly high that he abandoned his planned cross-cultural comparison.
The experiment emerged during a period when psychologists were grappling with fundamental questions about human nature and moral responsibility. Could ordinary individuals, when placed in certain circumstances, commit acts they would normally find morally reprehensible? The Milgram experiment 1963 sought to answer this critical question through controlled laboratory conditions.
Methodology and Design of the Milgram Experiment 1963
Experimental Setup and Participants
The original Milgram experiment 1963 involved 40 male participants aged 20-50, recruited from the New Haven community through newspaper advertisements. Participants came from diverse backgrounds, including postal clerks, high school teachers, salesmen, engineers, and laborers, ensuring a representative cross-section of the population.
The experimental setting was carefully designed to create an atmosphere of scientific authority. Conducted at Yale University’s impressive campus, the study took place in a laboratory equipped with what appeared to be sophisticated scientific equipment. This prestigious setting played a crucial role in establishing the experimenter’s credibility and authority.
The Shock Generator Apparatus
Central to the Milgram experiment 1963 was an imposing shock generator machine, meticulously crafted to appear authentic. The device featured 30 lever switches arranged horizontally, with voltage levels clearly marked from 15 to 450 volts in 15-volt increments. Verbal designations accompanied the switches, ranging from “Slight Shock” to “Danger: Severe Shock,” with the final two switches ominously marked “XXX”.
When activated, the machine produced convincing audio-visual effects: bright red pilot lights illuminated, electric buzzing sounds filled the room, a blue “voltage energizer” light flashed, and the voltage meter swung dramatically to the right. These carefully engineered details ensured that no participant suspected the device was merely a simulation.
Experimental Procedure
Each participant was assigned the role of “teacher” in what they believed was a learning and memory experiment. The “learner,” actually a confederate named Mr. Wallace, was strapped to an electric chair in an adjacent room. The teacher’s task was to read word pairs to the learner and administer electric shocks for incorrect responses, increasing the voltage with each mistake.
To enhance credibility, each participant received a genuine 45-volt shock from the machine before beginning. This sample shock, administered to their wrist, convinced participants of the generator’s authenticity.
The experimenter, dressed in a gray lab coat symbolizing scientific authority, provided standardized verbal prods when participants hesitated: “Please continue,” “The experiment requires that you continue,” “It is absolutely essential that you continue,” and “You have no other choice; you must go on”.
Results and Findings of the Milgram Experiment 1963
Shocking Statistics
The results of the Milgram experiment 1963 defied all expert predictions. Before conducting the study, Milgram surveyed psychiatrists who predicted that only 0.1% of participants would administer maximum shocks, expecting most to refuse between 150-300 volts.
The actual findings were dramatically different:
- 100% of participants continued to at least 300 volts
- 65% of participants (26 out of 40) administered the full 450 volts
- Only 14 participants stopped before reaching maximum voltage

These statistics revealed that ordinary individuals, when placed in an authoritative experimental context, would inflict what they believed to be severe pain on innocent strangers.
Participant Behavior and Stress Responses
Despite the high obedience rates, participants in the Milgram experiment 1963 displayed clear signs of psychological distress. Observed behaviors included sweating, trembling, stuttering, nervous laughter, and in one extreme case, a seizure due to severe stress. Many participants bit their lips, dug fingernails into their flesh, and exhibited uncontrollable laughter—all indicators of intense internal conflict.
Importantly, every participant questioned the procedure at some point, and many openly pleaded to stop or offered to return their payment rather than continue. This demonstrated that obedience did not stem from callousness or sadism, but from powerful situational pressures that overrode personal moral convictions.

Interpretation of Variables in the Milgram Experiment 1963
Independent Variables
The Milgram experiment 1963 and its subsequent variations manipulated several key independent variables to understand factors influencing obedience:
Authority Figure Proximity: The physical presence and distance of the experimenter significantly affected compliance rates. When instructions were given by telephone from another room, obedience dropped to 20.5%.
Victim Proximity: In the “Touch Proximity Condition,” participants were required to force the learner’s hand onto a shock plate after 150 volts, reducing obedience to 30%.
Location Prestige: Moving the experiment from Yale University to run-down offices in Bridgeport reduced obedience from 65% to 47.5%.
Authority Legitimacy: When the experimenter was replaced by an ordinary person in everyday clothes rather than a lab coat, obedience plummeted to 20%.
Social Support: The presence of disobedient confederates dramatically reduced compliance, with obedience falling to just 10% when participants witnessed others refusing to continue.
Dependent Variable
The primary dependent variable in the Milgram experiment 1963 was the degree of obedience, measured by the highest voltage level participants were willing to administer before refusing to continue. This quantifiable measure allowed researchers to compare obedience rates across different experimental conditions and variations.
Ethical Implications and Controversies Surrounding the Milgram Experiment 1963
Major Ethical Violations
The Milgram experiment 1963 raised unprecedented ethical concerns that fundamentally transformed psychological research standards. Several critical violations emerged:
Lack of Informed Consent: Participants were deliberately misled about the study’s true nature. While told it examined learning and punishment, the actual focus on obedience and authority was concealed, preventing genuine informed consent.
Psychological Harm: The experiment caused significant distress, anxiety, guilt, and potential lasting trauma. Some participants experienced severe emotional reactions that persisted well beyond the laboratory session.
Deception and Manipulation: The use of confederates, fake shock machines, and scripted responses constituted extensive deception. This violated participants’ right to truthful information about their involvement.
Coercive Environment: The experimenter’s verbal prods and authoritative presence created pressure that undermined participants’ ability to withdraw freely.
Inadequate Debriefing: Post-experimental debriefing procedures were insufficient to address the psychological harm caused. Many participants remained confused about the study’s true nature even after supposed debriefing.
Impact on Research Ethics Standards
The controversies surrounding the Milgram experiment 1963 catalyzed major reforms in psychological research ethics. These included:
- Establishment of Institutional Review Boards (IRBs) to evaluate research proposals
- Stricter informed consent requirements
- Enhanced protection for vulnerable populations
- Comprehensive debriefing procedures
- Greater emphasis on risk-benefit analysis
Modern Replications and Contemporary Relevance
Burger’s 2009 Partial Replication
Jerry Burger’s 2009 partial replication of the Milgram experiment 1963 demonstrated the study’s enduring relevance. Burger conducted a modified version that stopped at 150 volts—the point where learners first protested—to address ethical concerns while maintaining scientific validity.
The results showed that 70% of participants were willing to continue beyond 150 volts, closely mirroring Milgram’s original findings. This suggested that despite increased awareness of the original study and stronger ethical guidelines, human susceptibility to authority pressure remains largely unchanged.
Methodological Improvements
Modern replications of the Milgram experiment 1963 incorporate significant ethical safeguards:
- Strict screening procedures to exclude vulnerable participants
- Enhanced debriefing protocols with psychological support
- Modified stopping points to prevent extreme distress
- Transparent disclosure of deception after completion
- Follow-up assessments to ensure participant well-being
Lessons for Research Ethics Students
Understanding Power Dynamics
The Milgram experiment 1963 provides crucial insights for research ethics students about power imbalances between researchers and participants. The study demonstrates how scientific authority, institutional prestige, and situational pressures can compromise participant autonomy and voluntary consent.
Research ethics students must recognize that ethical conduct extends beyond formal procedures to encompass respect for human dignity, careful consideration of potential harm, and ongoing attention to participant welfare throughout the research process.
Balancing Scientific Value and Human Rights
The enduring debate surrounding the Milgram experiment 1963 illustrates the complex tension between advancing scientific knowledge and protecting research participants. While the study provided valuable insights into human behavior and obedience, these benefits came at significant cost to participant well-being.
Modern research ethics requires careful evaluation of whether potential scientific benefits justify any risks to participants, with a presumption favoring participant protection when benefits and risks are uncertain.
Contemporary Applications
Understanding the mechanisms revealed by the Milgram experiment 1963 remains relevant for addressing modern ethical challenges. The study’s insights apply to situations involving:
- Organizational compliance and whistleblowing
- Military and law enforcement training and conduct
- Healthcare hierarchies and patient advocacy
- Educational settings and student rights
- Digital platforms and user manipulation
Frequently Asked Questions
What was the main purpose of the Milgram experiment 1963?
The Milgram experiment 1963 was designed to investigate how far ordinary people would go in obeying authority figures who instructed them to perform acts conflicting with their personal conscience. Milgram specifically wanted to understand the psychological mechanisms behind obedience to authority and whether individuals would inflict harm on others when ordered to do so by legitimate authority figures.
How many people participated in the original Milgram experiment?
The original Milgram experiment 1963 included 40 male participants aged 20-50 years, recruited from the New Haven, Connecticut area through newspaper advertisements. These participants represented diverse occupational backgrounds, including postal clerks, teachers, salesmen, engineers, and laborers.
What percentage of participants administered the maximum shock?
In the Milgram experiment 1963, an alarming 65% of participants (26 out of 40) continued to administer shocks up to the maximum 450 volts. Additionally, 100% of participants continued to at least 300 volts, the point at which the learner first complained about heart problems and demanded to be released.
Why is the Milgram experiment considered unethical today?
The Milgram experiment 1963 is considered unethical by modern standards due to several serious violations: lack of informed consent (participants were deceived about the study’s true purpose), significant psychological harm and distress, extensive use of deception, coercive pressure to continue despite participants’ wishes to stop, and inadequate debriefing procedures. These issues led to major reforms in research ethics guidelines.
Has the Milgram experiment been replicated in modern times?
Yes, the most notable modern replication was conducted by Jerry Burger in 2009. This partial replication stopped at 150 volts to address ethical concerns while still providing meaningful data. Burger’s study found that 70% of participants were willing to continue beyond this point, suggesting that obedience to authority remains a powerful force in modern society.
What were the key variables that influenced obedience in the study?
Several factors significantly influenced obedience rates in the Milgram experiment 1963 variations: proximity of the authority figure (closer presence increased obedience), proximity to the victim (closer contact decreased obedience), legitimacy of the setting (prestigious Yale location increased compliance), presence of disobedient models (witnessing others disobey dramatically reduced compliance), and the authority figure’s appearance and credibility.
What safeguards are used in modern psychological research?
Modern psychological research incorporates numerous safeguards developed partly in response to studies like the Milgram experiment 1963: mandatory institutional review board approval, comprehensive informed consent procedures, participant right to withdraw without penalty, thorough risk-benefit analysis, enhanced debriefing protocols, special protections for vulnerable populations, and ongoing monitoring of participant welfare throughout studies.
The Milgram experiment 1963 continues to serve as both a landmark scientific achievement and a cautionary tale about the ethical responsibilities inherent in psychological research. Its findings about human obedience to authority remain profoundly relevant, while its ethical shortcomings have helped establish the rigorous standards that protect research participants today. For research ethics students, understanding both the scientific contributions and ethical failures of this seminal study provides essential insights into the ongoing challenge of balancing scientific progress with human dignity and welfare.
Read about the Psychology’s Most Controversial Study The Stanford Prison Experiment
[…] Zimbardo’s hypothesis centered on the belief that situational variables would prove more influential than individual personality traits in determining behavior. He theorized that the imposed social roles and environment of a prison would dominate participants’ individual personalities, causing them to exhibit more extreme behaviors than they would under normal circumstances.also read about The Milgram Experiment 1963 […]
[…] The milgram Experiment 1963 and The Stanford Prison […]